Evan Gordon “A mind-body network alternates with effector-specific regions in primary motor cortex”
March 08, 2023Information
- ID
- 9612
- To Cite
- DCA Citation Guide
Transcript
- 00:06Hey, everybody.
- 00:06Thank you very much for coming today.
- 00:08I'm going to talk a little bit
- 00:10about some very interesting,
- 00:11very surprising findings we have
- 00:14observed recently using functional
- 00:16connectivity in the primary motor plants.
- 00:19So primary motor cortex,
- 00:21everybody's just favorite part of the brain,
- 00:24really most interesting part of the brain.
- 00:29No, it's it's not right is kind of boring.
- 00:33We all care about frontal cortex,
- 00:35we care about lateral parietal cortex.
- 00:38And the reason is that we kind of already
- 00:40know how the motor cortex is organized.
- 00:43How can we hide this in some way?
- 00:53Already it's already
- 00:54minimized by this. Right, so.
- 00:59Yeah.
- 01:03Yeah, we already know how the
- 01:05motor cortex is organized.
- 01:06We know that the motor cortex
- 01:08is organized as a homunculus,
- 01:11and we've known this for a long time.
- 01:13The homunculus was first described
- 01:15by Wilder Penfield and then 1930s.
- 01:17And the basic idea that.
- 01:20Almost everybody here probably knows is
- 01:22that their motor representations organized
- 01:24as a linear progression of body parts.
- 01:27They just kind of run up your body
- 01:29from your foot on the dorsal medial
- 01:32portion of your primary motor cortex,
- 01:34wrapping around to your your your body,
- 01:39your shoulder, your elbow, your hand,
- 01:41and then going to your eyes,
- 01:44your face down in the lateral ventral
- 01:48lateral portion of the primary motor cortex.
- 01:51You know this is the first thing
- 01:52that we if this isn't the first
- 01:54thing that we all learn in first
- 01:55year introductory neuroscience class.
- 01:57It's it's the first thing we all remember.
- 01:59Right.
- 01:59And the reason this is the first
- 02:01thing we all remember is that this is
- 02:03incredibly cool and salient right.
- 02:04Like I mean some things you learn
- 02:07in intro it's like Oh well neurons
- 02:10have like these dendrites and axons.
- 02:12But this little man in my brain that is
- 02:15cool and I can remember that it's salient.
- 02:17It's it's really sticky concept, right.
- 02:20Like and so.
- 02:21This idea has really stuck it.
- 02:24It's been really influential,
- 02:26and it's lasted since Penfield
- 02:27first described this in the 1930s.
- 02:29Of course,
- 02:30Penfield techniques were really crude.
- 02:32You know,
- 02:33he was doing intraoperative
- 02:36direct cortical stimulation.
- 02:37Is electrodes were particularly precise.
- 02:40And you know how he marked out,
- 02:42you know which motion he got when he
- 02:44stimulated which part of the body?
- 02:46He had little pieces of paper
- 02:48that he stuck onto the cortex.
- 02:49And that's how he sort of kept track, right?
- 02:51And then he would write it
- 02:53all down in a notebook.
- 02:54So it's pretty crude by today's standards,
- 02:57but he got it right.
- 02:59Did he?
- 03:02We obviously have, I think more
- 03:04advanced ways to map the brain now,
- 03:07especially using F MRI and
- 03:11F MRI basically has.
- 03:14Ratified the homunculus,
- 03:16although that's been more viewed as,
- 03:19oh, you know,
- 03:19F MRI is showing us the right thing,
- 03:21then you know,
- 03:22the homunculus is clearly right because
- 03:24MRI says musculus is true, obviously.
- 03:28So you know,
- 03:28like if you do motor tasks,
- 03:30you can clearly see this organization
- 03:32where there's foot representation
- 03:33in the dorsomedial part,
- 03:35there's hand representation
- 03:35in the dorsal lateral part,
- 03:37there's face representation in
- 03:39the ventrolateral part and resting
- 03:41state shows us this as well.
- 03:43Thomas's networks back in 2011 had
- 03:46this clear segregation between at
- 03:48least the face and the the arm slash foot.
- 03:51We've shown that you can actually
- 03:53segregate foot from arm and this is,
- 03:55this is all really consistent with the phone.
- 04:02Of course, the idea that there is the human,
- 04:04let's say, get it like all of
- 04:06these different body parts are
- 04:07represented in slightly different
- 04:08places in primary motor cortex.
- 04:09That suggests that there's probably
- 04:11these functional subdivisions within
- 04:13primary motor cortex within these big,
- 04:15you know, so far we've mostly seen,
- 04:16you know, this is the big hand arm area,
- 04:19this is the big leg foot area,
- 04:21but there's probably subdivisions
- 04:22within that, right?
- 04:25We and others, especially Rodrigo,
- 04:27have had a lot of success using what
- 04:29we call precision functional mapping,
- 04:31where we collect tons of data in individuals
- 04:33to get really good representation of
- 04:35brain organization in each person,
- 04:37and we've used this to describe
- 04:40subdivisions of large scale brain networks
- 04:43in these individuals. And, you know,
- 04:45there's been a lot of great work.
- 04:46You know, we, we've shown subdivisions in
- 04:48cool networks like the default network
- 04:50and we've shown subdivisions in really
- 04:52interesting portions of the brain,
- 04:54like like how the striatum is
- 04:56connected to the medial frontal cortex.
- 04:58So we said, OK, you know,
- 05:00this is an easy next step.
- 05:02Let's just use the same approach,
- 05:04use this, you know,
- 05:06precision functional mapping and
- 05:07individuals to look for the homuncular
- 05:09subdivisions in motor cortex.
- 05:10And this will be, you know,
- 05:11an easy straightforward result and,
- 05:13you know, just.
- 05:15Again,
- 05:15ratifying that you know mapping
- 05:17that you can really get precise
- 05:20network connections when you map
- 05:22stuff really well in individuals.
- 05:24Well, we didn't find that molecules,
- 05:27surprisingly.
- 05:28Here's what we did find when we ran a
- 05:31series of functional connectivity seeds
- 05:34down the precentral gyrus and here.
- 05:37So here's 6 seeds right running
- 05:39from #1 and you can barely see it.
- 05:41Endorsing Media Group or yeah,
- 05:43dorsomedial and one running down
- 05:47to SEAT 6 invention lateral M1.
- 05:50We can pretty easily pick out exactly
- 05:53the foot and hand and face areas here.
- 05:56Number one is clearly foot #2 is hand,
- 05:59#5 is face, and if you do this
- 06:01is in the single individual.
- 06:02If you do,
- 06:03you know the classic HP motor
- 06:05task in this individual,
- 06:07which we've done,
- 06:08it lines up with these divisions
- 06:09just perfect.
- 06:12But what about seeds 2, four, and six?
- 06:15They look completely different,
- 06:17and in fact they don't look
- 06:19like 3 separate things.
- 06:20Instead, these three areas,
- 06:21seeds 2, four, and six,
- 06:23are all strongly connected with each other.
- 06:26There's this.
- 06:28We're distributing network
- 06:30inside primary motor cortex.
- 06:34These three seats.
- 06:37And if you don't really believe me,
- 06:39here is sort of all of the
- 06:41seeds in primary motor cortex.
- 06:42And as we run the seed from
- 06:45dorsal medial to ventral lateral,
- 06:47you can see it describe clearly what we
- 06:50call the effector specific regions, foot,
- 06:52hand, face as well as these three weird,
- 06:55we don't know what to call
- 06:57them inter effector regions.
- 06:58They're between the effector
- 07:00specific regions and they're there.
- 07:02Yeah there there's just sort of
- 07:04they're linked to each other,
- 07:05positioned between this is a weird thing.
- 07:11So that was in one person that I showed you,
- 07:13but we can see this these three intra
- 07:16effector regions strongly connected to
- 07:18each other in precentral gyrus in every
- 07:21single person we look at and I think
- 07:23participant 5 here participant 5 is the
- 07:26unusual because they also have strong
- 07:28connections with the motor face area,
- 07:30but they they still have this
- 07:32distributed thing in sort of the
- 07:34more dorsal part that should be like
- 07:36torso wish and the more ventral.
- 07:38That the lateral part that
- 07:40should be below the face area,
- 07:42it shows up really consistently.
- 07:44We have so much data in these people
- 07:46that we can split it out and we
- 07:48can do sort of within individual
- 07:50replications and the, you know,
- 07:52it replicates within individual and
- 07:55it is in all of the big group average
- 07:57datasets that we've ever looked at.
- 08:00So on the top we have the UK B,
- 08:024000 people from UK B second we have
- 08:054000 people from the ABC D third we have.
- 08:08Um, 800 people from the HCP.
- 08:10And on the bottom we have our old
- 08:13Washington University 120 data set.
- 08:14That's really old data,
- 08:15but even in this really old data,
- 08:17it was honestly,
- 08:18this data is not even field mapped.
- 08:22We didn't even realize that we
- 08:23should be field mapping it before
- 08:25we mapped it to the cortical surface
- 08:27and even this data you can see these
- 08:29three interdigitated intra effector
- 08:31regions inside primary motor board.
- 08:33So it's there.
- 08:36So you know we also have a good
- 08:38amount of pediatric data and we're
- 08:40starting to look at position functional
- 08:42mapping in pediatric populations.
- 08:44So we said how when does this emerge,
- 08:48when does this,
- 08:49these three inter affected regions,
- 08:50when do they emerge,
- 08:51it's just just in adults.
- 08:53Well it's in adults.
- 08:54We found it in nine year olds,
- 08:57you know we knew it was in nine year
- 08:59olds because it's so clear in a CD data.
- 09:02He found it in on 11 month old.
- 09:05Really easily,
- 09:05really easy to find it in an 11
- 09:07month old is not quite as strong
- 09:08in this 11 month old,
- 09:09but it's it's there on the top there.
- 09:12Not present in any unit in a neonate.
- 09:15We see all of these motor
- 09:18regions very broad smeared.
- 09:19There's not hard borders between any of them,
- 09:21and most critically there's not
- 09:24any distributed connectivity within
- 09:26precentral gyrus in this NEO.
- 09:28So it seems to this seems is
- 09:30not present at birth,
- 09:31but it seems to emerge as these
- 09:32cortical areas start forming
- 09:33in the first year of life.
- 09:37Here's a weird thing.
- 09:38These three inter effective regions,
- 09:40this network really strongly connected with
- 09:43the singular opercular control network.
- 09:45A singular perculiar network
- 09:46has a million different names.
- 09:48We call it single circular.
- 09:49It's also called the salience
- 09:50network in I think Thomas masks the
- 09:53ventral attention network, right?
- 09:55It's a it's an attention network.
- 09:57It's a it's a network that does top
- 10:00down control, it maintains goals,
- 10:01it helps set goals.
- 10:03It it does error processing,
- 10:05it has insular components that are linked to.
- 10:08That sort of do like pain registration
- 10:10and it's really strongly connected here.
- 10:13You can see in in these dorsal medial
- 10:15aspect that that's the SMA and the
- 10:18dorsal anterior singulate cortex.
- 10:20These are portions of the single
- 10:22particular network really strongly
- 10:23connected to these intra effector regions.
- 10:26And if we say OK,
- 10:28which parts of the brain are more
- 10:29connected to the interior of the
- 10:31three interactor regions than
- 10:32any of our hand foot mounters.
- 10:34So,
- 10:35so how are these interfactory
- 10:37regions different?
- 10:38From the effector specific regions
- 10:40we get basically exactly the
- 10:41singular curricular network.
- 10:43It traces the outlines of the single
- 10:45curricular network very closely,
- 10:47even with strong connectivity
- 10:49in anterior prefrontal cortex.
- 10:51In primary motor cortex being
- 10:53connected really strongly to
- 10:55that's that's lateral area 10.
- 10:57That's weird.
- 10:59But here we see the quantification.
- 11:01We look across all the different brain nodes.
- 11:03Singular is the one that has the
- 11:05biggest difference between the inter
- 11:08effectors and the effector specific networks.
- 11:10If we sort of visualize these
- 11:13relationships as a graph,
- 11:14we can see that where the single
- 11:16particular network is in purple
- 11:18down there on the left,
- 11:19the affector specific areas are in orange,
- 11:23blue and green on the right.
- 11:25These these intra effector regions
- 11:28are positioned between the single
- 11:30particular control and these
- 11:32classic hands like mountains.
- 11:34So is this.
- 11:36This is how control gets into motor systems.
- 11:41So here's a visualization of this,
- 11:44another visualization of the singular effect.
- 11:46Right here we have these are all
- 11:49of the very highly sampled high,
- 11:51high data individuals we have.
- 11:53Each one of them is represented as one dot.
- 11:56This sort of purplish represents
- 11:58the connectivity with the single
- 12:00particular network in each individuals
- 12:03interactor network and each individual.
- 12:05Also on the right you can see their foot,
- 12:08hand and mouth and there's you.
- 12:10So you can see this sort of paired effect.
- 12:12There's lines between each
- 12:13individual's interfactory network
- 12:15and their foot handed out,
- 12:17and you can see that the interactor in
- 12:19every individual is higher than every foot,
- 12:21hand mouth area.
- 12:23Because all lines go down,
- 12:25but it's not just singular particular
- 12:26network that we're seeing this
- 12:28difference and there's actually a
- 12:29lot of different ways that these
- 12:31interfactory regions are different
- 12:32from the foot hand mountains, so.
- 12:34Yeah.
- 12:35The Intersector network has stronger
- 12:38connectivity to the middle insulin
- 12:40than the foot, hand and mouth areas.
- 12:43The inner Effector network has stronger
- 12:45connectivity with the cerebellar vermis.
- 12:47The Interfactory network has stronger
- 12:49connectivity with the dorsal posterior
- 12:50containment, which is supposed to be
- 12:52the motor portion of the containment.
- 12:55The Interfactory network has
- 12:56stronger connectivity with a
- 12:57number of thalamic regions.
- 12:58Basically, most of motors values
- 13:00the central medial values,
- 13:02the ventral intermediate nucleus
- 13:04and the ventral posterior
- 13:06medial nucleus of the thalamus.
- 13:09The Interfactory network has much
- 13:11weaker connectivity with adjacent S1.
- 13:13This is a classic finding that M1
- 13:16is really strongly functionally
- 13:18connected to adjacent S1,
- 13:20but it's it's not really true with the
- 13:23inter effectors and you can go back well.
- 13:27If we go back and and sort of look at them,
- 13:30you can see that they're really in.
- 13:33The precentral gyrus they don't really.
- 13:35The top one extends a bit
- 13:37to post central gyrus,
- 13:38but the the the middle one and the
- 13:40inferior one really don't extend
- 13:42into post central gyrus very much.
- 13:44So they seem to be divorced
- 13:48somewhat from semantic sensation.
- 13:51We can move away from
- 13:52functional connectivity.
- 13:53We can look at structure.
- 13:54The inner effectors have lower cortical
- 13:56thickness than any of the foot,
- 13:57hand,
- 13:58mouth reactions.
- 13:58In three of our subjects we
- 14:00had diffusion imaging,
- 14:01really high amounts of diffusion imaging.
- 14:03We looked at fractional
- 14:05anisotropy right under cortex.
- 14:07The Interfactory networks has
- 14:09higher FA right under Cortex.
- 14:11This one I don't understand.
- 14:12I haven't figured out what this means yet,
- 14:14but it means something I don't know.
- 14:17And the inner effector network,
- 14:18we also looked at at Mylan
- 14:20mapping the T1T2 ratio,
- 14:22the intersector networks have
- 14:24lower myelin intracortical myelin
- 14:26content than the hand area,
- 14:28higher than the foot area,
- 14:29about the same as the back there, so.
- 14:31Overall, there's just,
- 14:32this is a completely different system.
- 14:35There's just a ton of different ways,
- 14:37ton of different connections kind
- 14:38of structure that suggest that
- 14:40this is a completely different
- 14:42system from our classic foot,
- 14:43hand, mouth things.
- 14:46So how does this make sense in the context
- 14:48of the homunculus that we've all learned?
- 14:51Well, the only way to answer that
- 14:53question is to go map the homunculus.
- 14:55So in two of our really
- 14:58highly sampled individuals,
- 14:59we conducted a an extensive series
- 15:01of homuncular mapping tasks.
- 15:02This is basically modeled after
- 15:04the HP motor task, which does,
- 15:05you know, move your hand, move your,
- 15:07move your collector foot, wiggle your tongue.
- 15:10Except we didn't just do those.
- 15:12We did toes, ankle, knee,
- 15:13which is dominant shoulder,
- 15:14elbow, wrist, hand, eyelid.
- 15:16High round nostrils jaw swallowing
- 15:18tongue and we had each subject
- 15:20run 64 different 5 minute tests.
- 15:23So this is the map of motor preference.
- 15:27This is placed onto a flat map.
- 15:29The green area is the lower limb area.
- 15:33That what we were talking
- 15:34before about this the foot area.
- 15:36The blue area is generally the hand
- 15:39area and the yellow, the orange.
- 15:42Yellow is generally the face area
- 15:45and what you can see here is that
- 15:47it actually does not look linear.
- 15:49It does not look like there is a linear
- 15:52progression from foot to face instead.
- 15:54Within each of these three
- 15:56separate fields, the foot field,
- 15:58the hand field in the face field,
- 16:01it looks like there's this
- 16:02concentric organization where in
- 16:04the middle of this field we have
- 16:06the most distal body part, right.
- 16:08So that would be the toes in the foot area,
- 16:10the hand in the in the upper arm
- 16:13area and the tongue in the face area.
- 16:16And then we have sort of concentric
- 16:18they it's a little messy,
- 16:19but you can see it most clearly in
- 16:21sort of the say the foot area over on.
- 16:24On the the.
- 16:27Left hemisphere,
- 16:28we have these concentric bands
- 16:30of representation where you
- 16:32have the most distal area,
- 16:33then proximal area around that,
- 16:35proximal area around that and
- 16:37then oh and you can see here's a
- 16:39representation for instance I've
- 16:41fit some curves to and I'll show
- 16:43you these curves in more detail.
- 16:45I've sort of fit some curves
- 16:48along along M1 extending from
- 16:50the dorsal portion down to the
- 16:52ventral portion on the X axis.
- 16:54Here you can see activation
- 16:56on the the Y axis.
- 16:57Position along M1 and what I'm
- 16:59doing is I am looking at activation
- 17:02strength at each position along
- 17:05this dorsal ventral axis and I fit.
- 17:08I fit one or two peak Gaussian
- 17:12curves here to represent like is
- 17:14there one peak is there to beacon?
- 17:15Whichever whichever fit is better,
- 17:17I represent and you can see that there
- 17:20are these dual peak curves for every
- 17:22motion for every one of these hand motion,
- 17:25wrist motion,
- 17:26elbow, shoulder.
- 17:28Abdominal and they are progressively
- 17:30farther and farther away from
- 17:32the hand from the hand peak,
- 17:34the most distal body part area.
- 17:39So yeah,
- 17:39these distal party body parts are in
- 17:41the center of these of the three fields,
- 17:43the proximal body parts are
- 17:45progressively surrounding them.
- 17:46What about the inner effectors?
- 17:47These fields intersect in the inner effects.
- 17:50It's like where the as the fields expand,
- 17:53they run into each other in
- 17:54the inner effectors.
- 17:57The cool part of this is this is completely
- 18:00violating pennfields among this, right?
- 18:01But it's not completely violating
- 18:03what's been shown in the macaque and
- 18:05then ignored because it's not a good
- 18:07as good a story as though molecules.
- 18:09So back in 1978, Juan showed basically
- 18:12this exact same thing in macaques,
- 18:14where you have the shoulder
- 18:16representation that's outside the elbow,
- 18:18the elbow representation
- 18:18is outside the wrist,
- 18:20the wrist representation is
- 18:22outside the fingers.
- 18:23You get these dank house 2006,
- 18:26dumb strict 2005.
- 18:27The park you can date you know they
- 18:30even say explicitly look this is
- 18:33distal versus proximal representations
- 18:35here concentrically in the in the
- 18:38this is in the hand area here.
- 18:40So this isn't a new observation
- 18:43but it's certainly new to anybody
- 18:45like me who thought that there
- 18:47was a monoculus and M1.
- 18:51So what are the intra
- 18:52factors doing in all this?
- 18:54Well, the interactor regions,
- 18:55you know, I showed you before that
- 18:57they have a preference, they do,
- 18:59they have a motion preference,
- 19:00they have to the winner take
- 19:02all approach is required to make
- 19:04them have a motion preference.
- 19:05But actually if you look in more detail,
- 19:08they have integrated activation
- 19:10during movement of many different
- 19:13body parts and their activation tends
- 19:16to coincide with strong activation
- 19:18in the single opercular network and.
- 19:21Most especially in this SMA dorsland tier
- 19:23singular portion of the singular network,
- 19:25although other portions as well.
- 19:28So here for instance,
- 19:30this is what looks like it's the
- 19:33winner of body motion in many
- 19:35parts of the interceptor network,
- 19:36the abdominals, right?
- 19:37And you can see that these abdominal
- 19:39activations really nicely traced
- 19:40out the inner effector network.
- 19:42They're strongest in the most superior
- 19:44region of the inner effector network,
- 19:46but they are clearly present
- 19:48in the other regions as well,
- 19:50but they're also.
- 19:51Extensively present in the single
- 19:53Percoll network,
- 19:53outlined in purple.
- 19:57But if we look across many
- 20:00different movements and we look
- 20:02across the whole extent of M1,
- 20:04and this is a little bit busy,
- 20:06but this is basically the same
- 20:08idea as what I showed before,
- 20:10where we're tracing these curves of
- 20:13activation from dorsal to ventral,
- 20:16dorsal on the left, ventral on the right.
- 20:19And for each of these different motions,
- 20:21we say, OK, at what, how active is this?
- 20:25A portion of Cortex during this motion,
- 20:29and what you can see is that in the effector
- 20:32specific areas here in the dark green,
- 20:34in the blue and in the
- 20:37orange we have this effect,
- 20:39where their preferred body part
- 20:41motions are of course very strong.
- 20:43And everything else is very suppressed,
- 20:45right by contrasting the intra
- 20:47effector regions in the purple
- 20:49surrounded by the dotted right,
- 20:51everything is kind of act.
- 20:54There's nothing that's really
- 20:55being strongly suppressed.
- 20:56Everything is.
- 20:57Medium activated and there's
- 20:58there's some that are stronger
- 21:00and there's some that are weaker,
- 21:02but across all body parts,
- 21:03they're all exhibiting some
- 21:04degree of activation here.
- 21:09The other thing that we were thinking
- 21:11about when we're trying to figure
- 21:13out what this network is doing is,
- 21:15well, let's think about it.
- 21:16It has these strong connections
- 21:17with the single regular network,
- 21:19single circular network is,
- 21:20is this goal oriented network,
- 21:22it maintains task goals.
- 21:25Could these regions have anything
- 21:28to do with with goals or planning?
- 21:32So what we did is we conducted 10 different
- 21:34in in our two highly sample subjects,
- 21:36we conducted 10 different runs of a task
- 21:39that required very complex coordinated
- 21:41movement across the hands and feet.
- 21:43And critically,
- 21:44this task had separable phases where
- 21:47they executed the complex motion
- 21:49and before that they had a little
- 21:51bit of time to plan how they were
- 21:52going to execute the complex motion.
- 21:54This actually helped them execute
- 21:56it because the motions were like
- 21:58like rotate your your your hand
- 22:00counterclockwise while your foot is.
- 22:02You know,
- 22:02bending forward and backward
- 22:04students both at the same time.
- 22:05So there was this planning phase and
- 22:07there was this execution phase and what
- 22:09we found is that when we contrasted
- 22:10the plan versus execution phase,
- 22:12the effect is a little weak,
- 22:14but it was clearly stronger overall in
- 22:16the inner effector regions than in the
- 22:19effector specific regions that were
- 22:21actually going to execute the test.
- 22:24So it looks like these regions
- 22:26are doing some sort of.
- 22:28Action planning as well.
- 22:31And then the final thing that we
- 22:33wanted to do when we're thinking
- 22:35about what these regions might
- 22:36be doing is we wanted to see if
- 22:38they were present in mechanics.
- 22:40So we got a bunch of data from
- 22:44the the Prime DE data set this
- 22:47was from kindly sent to us by.
- 22:50By Mike Williams Group and we were we
- 22:53were poking around in these macaque
- 22:57resting state functional connectivity
- 22:59matrices, and interestingly,
- 23:00we found that this is a this
- 23:02is group average.
- 23:03The individual mechanics are much noisier
- 23:05than we can get in individual humans,
- 23:07but this is group average data.
- 23:08Interestingly,
- 23:08we found that if we place a seed
- 23:10in anterior singular cortex,
- 23:12this is about in in monkey terms,
- 23:16it's the singular motor air.
- 23:18The which singular motor area is it?
- 23:20It's the.
- 23:21Hoddle single Motor area if the anterior
- 23:24portion of the caudal singular motor area,
- 23:27we get something that really does kind of
- 23:29look like the single opercular network here,
- 23:31our classic single circular slash
- 23:33salience slash ventral attention network.
- 23:34It has this extension into the farms margin.
- 23:36All this of the cingulate gyrus back here,
- 23:38it has its representation
- 23:40in supramarginal gyrus,
- 23:41it has it has strong representation
- 23:44and critically it has distributed
- 23:46representations within primary
- 23:47motor cortex when we seed the
- 23:50macaque anterior cingulate cortex.
- 23:52And we think that this is as good a
- 23:56candidate as any for a homologue of this
- 23:58intro effector network that we're seeing.
- 24:00The cool thing is that if you
- 24:02look at the mechanic literature,
- 24:04if you look at the,
- 24:06the radio graphic tracing
- 24:08literature in the Cacs,
- 24:10Peter Strick has actually done a lot
- 24:12of work recently where he showed that
- 24:14there are distributed portions of
- 24:16primary motor cortex that project down to
- 24:18internal organs like the adrenal medula,
- 24:20like the stomach and the kidney here.
- 24:23And frankly these adrenal medula protections,
- 24:26to my eyes at least,
- 24:27they pretty much exactly line up with
- 24:30the locations where we are finding enter
- 24:33where we're finding distributed functional
- 24:35connectivity with anterior singular cortex.
- 24:37And it, it kind of makes sense
- 24:39because the you also have this sort of
- 24:42anterior cingulate projections down
- 24:43into atrium which medula as well.
- 24:45Now straight characterize these as a well.
- 24:49He has a paper called the Mind Body
- 24:51Connection and it's not crazy to think that.
- 24:54Because why would you have direct projections
- 24:58between these medial frontal control regions?
- 25:01Even in the cat they probably do some sort
- 25:04of pop down control and the adrenal medula,
- 25:06well I mean to me the and to to strike
- 25:09the most likely explanation is that they
- 25:12are helping this sort of allostatic.
- 25:15If you know the word this sort of allostatic
- 25:18regulation pre regular anticipatory
- 25:20regulation of your internal body states,
- 25:23you have to give them.
- 25:24Is required to do something
- 25:26dangerous or stressful,
- 25:27even when they're planning it
- 25:28before they start doing it.
- 25:30Even when they're planning it,
- 25:31they you have this ramping up
- 25:34of adrenal projections that is
- 25:36actually absolutely critical,
- 25:38so that when they start doing it,
- 25:40their adrenaline levels are already high.
- 25:43So.
- 25:43In summary,
- 25:45we have found this weird inter effective
- 25:49integrated interdigitated INTERFACTORY
- 25:50network in primary motor cortex
- 25:53is strongly connects to prefrontal
- 25:55single particular regions that we know
- 25:58initiate and maintain task goals.
- 26:00It Co activates with the single particular
- 26:02network across many different movements,
- 26:04most strongly to axial body muscles.
- 26:07But not only it seems to activate
- 26:10at least to some degree across
- 26:12many different movements.
- 26:14It's very strongly segregated from,
- 26:17and it functions completely different
- 26:19from these systems that we know that
- 26:22are critical for complex, precise,
- 26:24isolated motion of your effectors,
- 26:26of especially of your hands and
- 26:28your feet and your tongue.
- 26:30And we it seems to be to some degree
- 26:32involved in action planning and
- 26:34it may also project to internal
- 26:36organs such as the adrenal medula,
- 26:38the stomach and the kidneys.
- 26:40So what is it? What?
- 26:42How do we put all this together?
- 26:44What is all this?
- 26:45I I don't.
- 26:45I'm not going to claim that I have
- 26:47the final answer.
- 26:48But our best guess,
- 26:49what makes the most sense right
- 26:51now to us is that this is a system
- 26:53for integrated whole body action
- 26:55in the service of goals.
- 26:57You have goals that are generated
- 26:59in the single opercular network.
- 27:02Somehow your body needs to know
- 27:04about those goals so that when
- 27:06you start going to execute them,
- 27:08your body is ready to do that.
- 27:11This may this may involve complex
- 27:13goal directed actions that require
- 27:14whole body integration.
- 27:16This may be in particular the
- 27:18kind of action that
- 27:19this interfactory network
- 27:21is particularly good at.
- 27:23This is my guess.
- 27:24We can't do this in in F MRI.
- 27:26Of course we can't have somebody
- 27:27dance or play football in F MRI.
- 27:29But I think it's it's potentially
- 27:31reasonable to think that in that
- 27:33whole body actions may be more
- 27:35strongly represented in the inner
- 27:37effector network than they are in
- 27:39represented in these other inspectors.
- 27:41Specific regions that we know are really,
- 27:43really good.
- 27:43If what you need to do is play piano,
- 27:45you need to only move your hands.
- 27:47Great. Perfect for the motor hand area.
- 27:49But what if your hand movement
- 27:51has to be totally integrated with
- 27:53your mouth and your face movement?
- 27:55What if it needs to be integrated
- 27:57with your foot movements?
- 27:59Maybe you need a distributed
- 28:01integrated system,
- 28:02but we also think that maybe this
- 28:05system is performing anticipatory
- 28:07physiological changes so that planned
- 28:10actions can be appropriately executed.
- 28:12I have to give a big talk
- 28:14my my hands are sweating,
- 28:16I have butterflies in my stomach.
- 28:17These are somatic responses not to
- 28:20anything currently happening to you,
- 28:22but to your plans.
- 28:23I have to go to the bathroom.
- 28:26Why do I have to go to the bathroom when
- 28:28I shouldn't have to go to the bathroom?
- 28:30Except I have to give a talk in 5
- 28:32minutes and suddenly my body has sort
- 28:33of started ramping everything up.
- 28:35How does that happen?
- 28:36This is 1 potential mechanism.
- 28:39So we were calling this the
- 28:41mind body network for a while,
- 28:42but I I don't love that name.
- 28:44So we've moved to calling this a
- 28:47somatic cognitive Action Network.
- 28:49I don't love names.
- 28:50I don't love this name,
- 28:51but I think this is the name that
- 28:54captures the most of what we have here.
- 28:57So we do think, but regardless of the name,
- 29:00we do think that it's I think appropriate
- 29:02now to rethink Penfield homunculus.
- 29:05Instead of this sort of linear
- 29:07representation running from foot to face,
- 29:09instead we have these three
- 29:11separate effector specific fields
- 29:12that are not organized linearly.
- 29:15They are organized as concentric
- 29:17fields where they meet.
- 29:19You have this separate system
- 29:21where top down control is coming
- 29:23in not for isolated movements,
- 29:26but for control of your whole body.
- 29:29And we're really excited.
- 29:30We just found out last week this
- 29:32has been accepted at nature.
- 29:34It's now in press.
- 29:36So we're super excited about that.
- 29:39So that's all I have.
- 29:42Unless people I I also have
- 29:43so much stuff in this paper.
- 29:45You can go read the paper.
- 29:46There's a bunch of findings
- 29:47I had to leave out.
- 29:48But this is all I really
- 29:49have time for right now.
- 29:50So I'd like to acknowledge the
- 29:52huge number of people who put a
- 29:54ton of effort in a ton of data.
- 29:56You may have noticed it was like it was
- 29:58like 12 different datasets in this project.
- 30:01And so everybody who contributed data,
- 30:03everybody contributed,
- 30:04contributed effort,
- 30:05but especially Nico Dosenbach,
- 30:07Tim Laman,
- 30:07and Russell and Chavin did the bulk.
- 30:09Of the the work here.
- 30:12Thank you very much. Any questions?
- 30:19That's typical stuff.
- 30:20I really like this sort of
- 30:22curiosity driven science.
- 30:24We try to figure out the whole story we had.
- 30:26But my question is, are very curious,
- 30:29curiosity driven 1A couple years ago,
- 30:31my wife tried to get me to Pilates, yeah,
- 30:33which didn't go as well as obviously,
- 30:37but one of things you have to do is
- 30:39learn how to control parts of your body.
- 30:41And I'm just thinking about 11 and I wonder
- 30:43if you got a Pilates expert in there.
- 30:45About myself in this kind of you
- 30:47think that you'd see this kind of more
- 30:49sort of precise representations that
- 30:51were of the abdomen and the sort of
- 30:54abdominal system as you have to learn
- 30:56how to control that volitionally.
- 30:57I I think that's very plausible.
- 31:02And So what would you,
- 31:03what would you predict,
- 31:04I think you would predict that that
- 31:06the abdominal representations might
- 31:07be you might be able to start ice,
- 31:10they might start isolating a little bit
- 31:12more from this network potentially.
- 31:13Yeah, I think it's I think it's
- 31:15great hypothesis.
- 31:19Great talk. You know,
- 31:22given that the brain is sort
- 31:23of like billions of neurons,
- 31:25finally all different frequencies
- 31:26and different networks,
- 31:27and the bold signals,
- 31:28the bold signals.
- 31:29So it's not actually sensitive
- 31:31to those types of.
- 31:33Could it be that you've found a
- 31:35FMR bold signal specific enough
- 31:37place that doesn't necessarily
- 31:39refute the traditional funds
- 31:41based on their likelihood?
- 31:45So we've certainly thought about this,
- 31:48you know, how much of this
- 31:50is a weird F MRI artifact?
- 31:52So we're really heartened by
- 31:53the fact that there's these
- 31:54structural differences, yes.
- 32:00Right. But that's different.
- 32:04But that's that's a great point.
- 32:06It's, it's certainly the interpretation
- 32:08becomes a lot harder if you're
- 32:10saying that the bold signal is
- 32:12giving us something that is.
- 32:14Totally valid,
- 32:15but completely independent from.
- 32:17Also correct. You know,
- 32:21electric cortical stimulation things.
- 32:24That would be, I think that would be tough.
- 32:26It's, I'm not saying it's wrong,
- 32:27but I think that would be tough.
- 32:28However, we are certainly
- 32:29heartened by the fact that a,
- 32:30we have this structural data that
- 32:32also suggests there's something
- 32:33fundamentally different about these areas.
- 32:35BI did, I did not have a
- 32:37chance to put this in the talk.
- 32:39There was an interesting paper in
- 32:412020 by a French group that was
- 32:45basically redoing pending, right.
- 32:47And so they were doing,
- 32:48they were doing homuncular mapping
- 32:50with direct electrical stimulation
- 32:51and they had a nice big data set.
- 32:53It was like 100.
- 32:55People and they had multiple
- 32:57simulation sites per person and
- 32:59and they put everything in and
- 33:01I space it looks very beautiful.
- 33:03The interesting thing was they had a gap.
- 33:08When they sort of plotted things from
- 33:09the dorsal portion to the ventral portion,
- 33:11there was this gap where they
- 33:14didn't get any responses at all.
- 33:16And we went and plotted their responses.
- 33:19On to our break.
- 33:20We have this might have this slide.
- 33:21Hang on.
- 33:28Yeah, here we go. All right.
- 33:31So here is the figure from
- 33:33Rue at all on the lower left.
- 33:36So they have all of these different motions.
- 33:39And on the the Y axis
- 33:43here is distance along M1.
- 33:45And you can I have this little
- 33:47arrow pointing to the gap.
- 33:49They even point out this gap in their paper.
- 33:51They have this gap between
- 33:53the the hands in the face.
- 33:55And we took all of their simulations and
- 33:57we plotted them onto the cortical surface
- 33:59and that's what's being represented.
- 34:01In the middle portion here and under
- 34:03that we're showing that the HP
- 34:05Group average version of the Inter
- 34:07effector regions and you can see
- 34:09that this gap exactly lines up with
- 34:12the middle inter effector region.
- 34:14So there may be something about
- 34:16these interactive regions that don't
- 34:19respond to stimulation, right?
- 34:20And then there was also a recent
- 34:23paper that actually just it came
- 34:25out after our free print.
- 34:27This is a cool paper.
- 34:28No, I have this miss labeled.
- 34:31Sorry,
- 34:32this is not Courier.
- 34:40There we go. Yeah, this is a really
- 34:42cool paper by Jensen at all.
- 34:44And what they did is they,
- 34:46they actually heard about our,
- 34:48our results a little bit ahead
- 34:51of time and they went and had
- 34:53electrodes in approximately the
- 34:55same locations as our superior and
- 34:58our middle inner vector regions.
- 35:00And they looked at what did they do?
- 35:04Oh, they asked, this is awake.
- 35:07Awake nurse surgery.
- 35:08And so they were recording and they
- 35:11asked participants to move their hand,
- 35:14move their, move their feet,
- 35:16move their tongue.
- 35:17And these electrodes that were
- 35:20placed in the the equivalents
- 35:22of our superior and middle inner
- 35:25effector regions you can see here
- 35:27responded to all of the motions.
- 35:29Whereas the electrodes in the foot region
- 35:32only responded to foot stuff that's purple.
- 35:35The electrodes in the hand region
- 35:36only responded to hand stuff.
- 35:37That's read the electrodes in
- 35:39the inner effector equivalent
- 35:41regions responded to everything.
- 35:42So I think that there is,
- 35:45I think it's not just bold is different.
- 35:48I think it's this has been missed.
- 35:53Some really amazing amount of
- 35:55work and then great results.
- 35:572 to thoughts or comments there.
- 36:00Once, I'm not quite sure I agree
- 36:02on the cognitive part because maybe
- 36:05it's just the difference between
- 36:07literal physiological effort but
- 36:08high coordination in distant
- 36:10body parts whereas anything else
- 36:12where you need your whole body.
- 36:13I mean you mentioned sports,
- 36:14but evolutionary it's probably
- 36:16more like hunting and stuff,
- 36:17you just need a lot more logical ramp up.
- 36:21Maybe that's that's actually the
- 36:22thing not so much the the cognitive
- 36:24part that's what we experience.
- 36:26But in terms of evolution,
- 36:27it's really the overall body ramp up.
- 36:29To to have this demanding whole
- 36:33body movements that's sort of
- 36:34the one thought possibly.
- 36:36I mean this is these are these are.
- 36:40I mean a lot of this.
- 36:43I hope your brain body is actually yeah.
- 36:48A lot of these ideas are are sort of
- 36:51really driven by the idea that we
- 36:53have this very strong connectivity
- 36:54so far anterior and medial medial
- 36:57prefrontal cortex right like this is
- 36:59this dorsal anterior singular cortex.
- 37:01It's it to me it would be really
- 37:03surprising if there was not some
- 37:05cognitive component of this.
- 37:06I'm not saying this is like pure
- 37:08cognition you know like ethereal like
- 37:11you know totally divorced and body movement.
- 37:13But to me there is that is that
- 37:15is a region that is not a really
- 37:18a pure motor region.
- 37:19Right.
- 37:20It's it's a pretty controlled region
- 37:22and maybe control of actions,
- 37:25but it's a pretty controlling region.
- 37:26I think there's a couple
- 37:27of cognitive component,
- 37:28but I agree this needs to be
- 37:30worked out better.
- 37:31Additionally, the trick there,
- 37:32I mean if it yeah control that,
- 37:34it could also be just it's ramp up control,
- 37:37it doesn't minor second point thing
- 37:40all the way back to my work on S2,
- 37:42it may actually be that S1 in fact
- 37:45just three B is the only outlier
- 37:47and everything else is organized
- 37:50by proximal weather system.
- 37:53That's kind of what we didn't
- 37:54make any good big points about
- 37:56this one because it was stupid.
- 37:59All areas of two.
- 38:00It's always the approximate really.
- 38:02I think it's S1 as well.
- 38:05I, I don't,
- 38:05I I we didn't make any claims about this,
- 38:08but I believe it's also as one.
- 38:11We have time. We have one question to chat.
- 38:19Any ideas why they may be separated?
- 38:20Oh my God, this is a wonderful question.
- 38:23No, I think it's this is such
- 38:26an interesting question.
- 38:28Why would they be separated?
- 38:29If you have this system,
- 38:31why does it need to be
- 38:32distributed like this, right?
- 38:33Why don't you just have it all in one place?
- 38:36I've been thinking about this not just
- 38:38in the context of of this system,
- 38:40but also in the context of of like
- 38:43Rodrigo's results showing that that
- 38:46other networks across the cortex.
- 38:48Have this interdigitated property.
- 38:49Why would they have this
- 38:51interdigitated property?
- 38:51What does it do?
- 38:53I think that this is telling us something,
- 38:55but I don't know what yet.
- 38:56I think my best guess is that there
- 38:59are interactions that bold and
- 39:02especially functional connectivity.
- 39:03Bold doesn't really capture between the
- 39:06surrounding areas and the area in between,
- 39:08right?
- 39:09So you've got two infected regions
- 39:11surrounding surrounding the hand area
- 39:13and it to me it seems likely that they
- 39:16may be influencing that hand area.
- 39:18They're not driving its activity,
- 39:20but they're maybe influencing it via
- 39:22short range lateral connections.
- 39:24That's my best guess, but.
- 39:27I don't. I don't know.
- 39:28I think this is something
- 39:29that needs to be worked out.
- 39:31My question only relevance being curious
- 39:34about like is it separately maybe
- 39:36because like it's in the serpents.
- 39:39Have you ever tried looking into the
- 39:41whole space and see whether it's like
- 39:44still connected or like some printed.
- 39:46So I haven't done a lot of looking
- 39:48at these representations in volume,
- 39:51they're not connected with
- 39:52each other in motor cortex.
- 39:54They're too far apart in motor
- 39:55cortex to like actually be connected
- 39:57in volume like they're there,
- 39:58these are like.
- 39:59These are good 2 centimeters apart
- 40:01and Jason in some cases.