Emily Jacobs “Applying dense-sampling methods to reveal dynamic endocrine modulation of the nervous system”
March 08, 2023Information
- ID
- 9618
- To Cite
- DCA Citation Guide
Transcript
- 00:06Our next speaker is Professor Emily Jacobs,
- 00:10and she's joining us
- 00:12remotely from Santa Barbara.
- 00:14So you're not in the snow,
- 00:17hopefully you went surfing or something,
- 00:19and we're happy to have you.
- 00:21Thank you. Thank you so much.
- 00:25So please just interrupt if you can't hear
- 00:27me if something goes wrong with the mic.
- 00:30So I just want to start off by saying I am so
- 00:34disappointed that I can't be there in person,
- 00:37you know, thank you to the organizers,
- 00:39to Todd and the whole organizing
- 00:41committee for this conference.
- 00:42It looks incredible and I have.
- 00:45At least top three FOMO moments of my life.
- 00:48So I am shaking my fists at Air Canada for
- 00:51cancelling my flight at the last minute.
- 00:53I'm I'm grateful that my trainee,
- 00:55Laura, was able to make it and I'm just.
- 00:57I'm distraught that I
- 00:59can't be there with you.
- 01:01So with that,
- 01:02the title of this talk is applying
- 01:04dense sampling methods to reveal dynamic
- 01:06endocrine modulation of the nervous system.
- 01:12So if I ask you to close your
- 01:15eyes and imagine the human brain,
- 01:18you might imagine something
- 01:20that looks like this,
- 01:21sort of like a £3.00 squishy walnut.
- 01:25Um, or because you are. You know,
- 01:28the audience is mostly human brain imagers.
- 01:30Maybe you imagine something
- 01:32a little bit more like this.
- 01:35But as my buddy John Morrison likes to say,
- 01:39sometimes neuroscientists can be so
- 01:41taken by the unique capabilities and the
- 01:45complexities of the human brain that we
- 01:48forget something really important about it,
- 01:51which is that it's connected
- 01:53to the rest of the body.
- 01:55So if you were to ask a neuroendocrinologist
- 01:59or Evan Gordon to imagine the brain,
- 02:02you'd probably imagine
- 02:04something more like this, right?
- 02:06The brain, you know,
- 02:07in its full richness and its full
- 02:10connection to the human body.
- 02:11And the endocrine system is a
- 02:14perfect example of this kind of
- 02:16whole body or brain body integration.
- 02:18And in this talk I'm going to focus
- 02:21specifically on gonadal hormones,
- 02:23which are produced by the ovaries
- 02:26and testes and use the circulatory
- 02:28system as their superhighway.
- 02:30These are highly lipophilic hormones.
- 02:32They travel through the blood and they
- 02:36bind anywhere there are adequate receptors.
- 02:40So in short, the brain is an endocrine organ.
- 02:44We've got 20 years of animal studies
- 02:47suggesting that gonadal hormones,
- 02:49in other words sex steroid hormones,
- 02:52are critical neuromodulators
- 02:53of learning and memory.
- 02:55And this cartoon here just shows
- 02:58the hypothalamic,
- 02:59pituitary gonadal or HPG axis.
- 03:02It's more famous sister is the HP access.
- 03:06But this HPG axis represents the
- 03:09interaction between our brain and our
- 03:12reproductive endocrine system, right?
- 03:14So there's this tightly coordinated
- 03:17neuroendocrine cascade in which about
- 03:191500 neurons in the hypothalamus.
- 03:22These are GNRH,
- 03:23Eric and Atrophin releasing hormone neurons.
- 03:25They regulate the production
- 03:28via the pituitary.
- 03:29They regulate the production of gonadal
- 03:32steroids or sex steroids from your gonads.
- 03:35So.
- 03:36Estrogen, progesterone,
- 03:37testosterone from the ovaries and testes.
- 03:40And these steroid hormones,
- 03:42as I mentioned,
- 03:43they're highly lipophilic.
- 03:44They travel through the bloodstream
- 03:45to bind to receptors in different
- 03:47tissues throughout the body.
- 03:49And we know that the brain is a major
- 03:52target organ for these sex steroid hormones.
- 03:54And in particular regions of
- 03:57the medial temporal lobe,
- 03:58including the hippocampus proper
- 04:00and the surrounding tissue and
- 04:02the prefrontal cortex contains
- 04:04significant populations of these
- 04:06steroid hormone receptors,
- 04:08and they may be particularly
- 04:10responsive to this. Hormone signaling.
- 04:15Some of that work comes from John Morrison.
- 04:17So he's the director of the National Primate
- 04:19Research Center at UC Davis right now,
- 04:20and he his lab showed a number of
- 04:23years ago now that estrogen receptor
- 04:26alpha R alpha is present in about
- 04:3050% of axo spinous synapses within
- 04:33the monkey prefrontal cortex.
- 04:35And this is true in young and
- 04:38in aged animals.
- 04:39And if you look at the abundance of
- 04:41sort of if you look at individual
- 04:43differences between animals just in
- 04:45a kind of crude correlational sense,
- 04:47we know that the abundance of synaptic
- 04:49ER alpha within the prefrontal
- 04:52cortex correlates with performance
- 04:54on a delayed response task.
- 04:56So this was some of the some of the
- 04:58early evidence at least within the
- 05:00non human primate literature that's
- 05:01something about estrogen receptor
- 05:03signaling may have something to do with
- 05:05these higher order cognitive functions.
- 05:08And this comes on the heels of work in
- 05:10the 90s, which I'll get to in a minute,
- 05:12showing effects of estrogen
- 05:15in the hippocampus.
- 05:16But but really what?
- 05:17What was groundbreaking about
- 05:19this was that if you crack open
- 05:21a neuroendocrinology textbook?
- 05:22From the 80s or even early 90s,
- 05:24all of it would be describing estrogen
- 05:27action within the hypothalamus or
- 05:29these really kind of routine um
- 05:31reproductive behaviors like lordosis.
- 05:33But we now know that these steroid
- 05:36hormones are acting in extra
- 05:38hypothalamic sites in the cortex.
- 05:42Umm, so these kinds of careful studies
- 05:44have been carried out in animal models.
- 05:46In humans, we know a lot less.
- 05:49So down here at UCSB, my lab is trying to
- 05:52get a toehold on how gay Natal hormones
- 05:55shape aspects of the human brain.
- 05:57And I don't have time to talk
- 05:59about most of what we're up to.
- 06:00So in this talk,
- 06:01I'm really just focusing on some
- 06:03of our dense sampling studies that
- 06:05are trying to map the brain across
- 06:07major in our endocrine transitions.
- 06:09For example,
- 06:11across the menstrual cycle and now pregnancy.
- 06:15So if you know nothing else about
- 06:18the endocrine system,
- 06:19know this hormone secretion varies over time,
- 06:23right. It's not static.
- 06:24There's there are these sort of pulsatile
- 06:27releases over different time scales.
- 06:29And one of these time scales in which
- 06:31we see these very sort of canonical
- 06:34changes in hormone production is
- 06:36over the menstrual cycle shown here,
- 06:38right, where there are these,
- 06:39again, this is just a cartoon,
- 06:40right,
- 06:40but there are these sort of increases in
- 06:43estrogen and these very sort of metronomic.
- 06:45Rhythms followed by an increase
- 06:47in progesterone.
- 06:49In the world of a rat or a mouse,
- 06:51as shown here,
- 06:52they don't have a menstrual cycle.
- 06:54They have something similar called
- 06:55an estrous cycle.
- 06:56This is happening over a four to
- 06:59five day period and really beautiful
- 07:01foundational studies conducted in
- 07:04the 1990s by Catherine Woolley
- 07:07was the first to link changes
- 07:10in hippocampal spine density to
- 07:13a rodent estrus cycle stage so
- 07:16the stage can be broken up into.
- 07:19Diestrus.
- 07:20When estrogen levels are very low,
- 07:22this is the start of the cycle and you
- 07:25can see fairly sparse spines within
- 07:27this is a C1 neuron within the hippocampus.
- 07:3024 hours later during proestrus
- 07:32when estradiol peaks.
- 07:34You don't even need statistics,
- 07:35right?
- 07:35Like just the the the graphics graph itself
- 07:38kind of tells you what you need to know.
- 07:40You see this proliferation of spines,
- 07:43and then the next day, following ovulation,
- 07:46estrogen levels fall and you see
- 07:48these spines again appear sparse.
- 07:52Now.
- 07:53One limitation of Catherine's
- 07:55experiments which is no fault of her
- 07:57own because they were done in the 90s,
- 07:59so she was limited to the
- 08:02technology of its time.
- 08:03One limitation is that these
- 08:05are based on Histology,
- 08:06so they are necessarily cross-sectional,
- 08:08right?
- 08:08You're sacking the animal,
- 08:09you're you're, you know, doing stains.
- 08:12These are camera lucida drawings.
- 08:14OK,
- 08:14so an open question is whether
- 08:17these morphological changes
- 08:18are evident across the estrous
- 08:21cycle in awake behaving animals.
- 08:27And so to answer that question,
- 08:28right now my lab is collaborating
- 08:31with Michael Gord's group at UCSB
- 08:33because they've developed super
- 08:35cool techniques for high resolution
- 08:37imaging of hippocampal neurons using
- 08:39two photon microscopy and awake mice.
- 08:42So they use this microprism so it's
- 08:46implanted kind of like a submarine
- 08:49Periscope flipped upside down,
- 08:51and they can image the hippocampal
- 08:53circuit at cellular resolution.
- 08:58So in a series of experiments
- 08:59led by Nora Wolcott,
- 09:01a graduate student in Mike's group,
- 09:04she imaged female mice every
- 09:0612 hours for eight days.
- 09:09This corresponds to two
- 09:11complete estrus cycles.
- 09:12And at each imaging session,
- 09:14she's performing vaginal
- 09:15cytology to stage the mice.
- 09:18This is all done blind,
- 09:19so she actually developed a machine
- 09:21learning classifier called Estrus net.
- 09:23So if anybody's doing it
- 09:25works for mice and rodents.
- 09:27If you want to stage animals
- 09:29and you don't know how,
- 09:30I I highly recommend this tool.
- 09:34So with longitudinal 2P imaging,
- 09:36she's able to track spines on
- 09:39apical dendrites of CA one
- 09:41neurons over several weeks.
- 09:43And I'll just note that these
- 09:45are the same segments of
- 09:47the dendrite every time.
- 09:49So it's not different sections,
- 09:50it's literally the same segment.
- 09:51So you can see new spines come and go.
- 09:57And consistent with
- 09:59Catharine's Classic result,
- 10:00Nora finds this increase in spine density
- 10:04during proestrus or this P period here.
- 10:08An increased pruning during estrus.
- 10:11So this band shown here,
- 10:13E and this ebbs and flows right?
- 10:15She sees it across the two cycles.
- 10:18Overall, there's about a 15% change in
- 10:21total spines from proestrus to estrus,
- 10:23which is, you know,
- 10:25potentially billions of synaptic connections.
- 10:28So it's clear. As sort of proof
- 10:31of concept that that you know,
- 10:33there's this modulation of spine
- 10:35density across the estrous cycle.
- 10:37But why? Right? So, So what?
- 10:40What? What are the consequences?
- 10:42Why is the brain organized in this way?
- 10:44Why are these rhythms happening?
- 10:48So umm, and don't hate me for
- 10:50showing all the rodent work
- 10:52because I'm so excited about this.
- 10:55It just came out.
- 10:55So bear with me and then I'll
- 10:57get to the human stuff.
- 10:58So the next step is really to
- 11:00see how these spine changes may
- 11:02influence functional properties
- 11:04of the hippocampal neurons.
- 11:06And so Nora just wrapped up experiments
- 11:08where she's imaging calcium activity
- 11:10and transgenic gcamp mice while they
- 11:13navigate in this floating circular chamber.
- 11:16So she can, so they're moving right?
- 11:19So she can map the response properties of C,
- 11:21A1 place fields overtime.
- 11:23So here's just a rendering of these chambers,
- 11:26and there's two different environments,
- 11:30let's call them A&B.
- 11:33So what you can see here in this panel,
- 11:37there are multiple place fields that respond
- 11:40to a particular part of the circular track,
- 11:43and here they've just been aligned to
- 11:45their preferred response in track a here.
- 11:47So you can see that these same
- 11:51cells respond very differently
- 11:53and track B this orange coating,
- 11:56but similarly when presented in track again.
- 12:00So you can look at 2 measures.
- 12:01So one is this measure of stability.
- 12:03So it's the similarity of the population
- 12:07response between A and a prime.
- 12:10Which we're coding here in dark blue,
- 12:12and you can also look at the remapping
- 12:14of place cells between two environments,
- 12:17so between A&B.
- 12:19And when estrogen is peaking in proestrus,
- 12:23so this panel here.
- 12:26We see great the greatest stability
- 12:30and the greatest remapping.
- 12:33Suggesting that these spine changes are are,
- 12:36you know,
- 12:37really having an effect on the
- 12:39functional output within this system.
- 12:42OK, what about in humans?
- 12:44You can we see, oh,
- 12:46and I'll just say, you know,
- 12:47we're following this up in collaboration
- 12:50with a group here at UCSB to do
- 12:53CRISPR to decrease the expression
- 12:55of specific sex hormone receptors to
- 12:57see if we can eliminate the effect
- 12:59and then and then we'll be done.
- 13:01OK. But what about in humans?
- 13:02So can we see morphological and functional
- 13:05changes across the menstrual cycle?
- 13:08So the lab had this crazy idea to image a
- 13:11person's brain every 24 hours for 30 days,
- 13:14collecting blood and brain scans and
- 13:16and mood scores at each time point.
- 13:21Because this is an informal conference,
- 13:22I'll show that, one editor later wrote.
- 13:25They're like old school NASA test pilots.
- 13:27They put themselves in an MRI
- 13:29scanner and drew their own
- 13:31blood every day for two months.
- 13:33That was a little colorful because
- 13:35we did have hire a certified
- 13:38phlebotomist and the first author,
- 13:40the test subject and the pioneer
- 13:42of this project, Laura Pritchett,
- 13:44is there in the audience.
- 13:46So big round of applause for her for giving
- 13:49herself over to science in a really.
- 13:51Dramatic way.
- 13:55OK, so why did we try to
- 13:57pull off this crazy study?
- 14:00Basically because cross-sectional
- 14:01studies that sample the brain
- 14:03at one time point can't tell
- 14:06us anything about how the brain
- 14:08changes day-to-day or week to week.
- 14:10And as I mentioned earlier,
- 14:12one central feature of the mammalian
- 14:15endocrine system is that hormone
- 14:17secretion varies over time.
- 14:19So during the average human
- 14:21menstrual cycle shown here,
- 14:22these are data from Laura.
- 14:25So we can see there's about an 8
- 14:28fold increase in estrogen which
- 14:30is plotted in this teal color.
- 14:32And there's actually about an 80
- 14:35fold increase in progesterone,
- 14:3780 fold change in progesterone
- 14:39plotted in blue.
- 14:40Note that these are on different scales.
- 14:43And the kinds of cross-sectional
- 14:45studies that are typically used in
- 14:47human brain imaging studies can't
- 14:49capture these dynamic endocrine changes.
- 14:51So we were really inspired by the
- 14:53dense sampling work pioneered by folks
- 14:55like Poldrack and the Midnight Scan Club,
- 14:58because that kind of intensely
- 15:01longitudinal imaging lends itself
- 15:03beautifully to our problem,
- 15:05which is trying to understand
- 15:07how these endocrine rhythms
- 15:09may be influencing the brain.
- 15:11And I think it's worth noting that
- 15:13for most women, for most of our lives,
- 15:15this ebb and flow is as steady as the tides.
- 15:19This pulse is almost like a vital sign,
- 15:21right?
- 15:22We know that these rhythmic changes Dr.
- 15:24Physiological functions.
- 15:25But nobody really knew how
- 15:27they shaped the brain,
- 15:29so the 28 me project was
- 15:32designed to to determine that.
- 15:34OK,
- 15:34so as I mentioned,
- 15:35the participant had daily blood draws,
- 15:39time locked and MRI every 24
- 15:42hours for 30 consecutive days
- 15:45across one complete cycle.
- 15:46And then she did the whole thing
- 15:48again a second time one year later.
- 15:54So the data that I'm going to show you first
- 15:57are from a 10 minute resting state scan.
- 16:00I'll note that she had very little motion,
- 16:04less than 130 microns on average each day.
- 16:08And to start out with so,
- 16:11so here our cortical parcellation INS,
- 16:13we're defined from the 415
- 16:15node Schafer Atlas plus the 15
- 16:18subcortical Harvard Oxford Atlas.
- 16:20And to begin with,
- 16:22we wanted to just test this
- 16:23straightforward hypothesis that
- 16:25whole brain resting state functional
- 16:28connectivity may be associated with
- 16:30intrinsic fluctuations in estradiol and
- 16:33progesterone in a time synchronous,
- 16:35so just day by day fashion.
- 16:40And sure enough that, you know,
- 16:41the first thing we observed is
- 16:43that these endocrine changes
- 16:44impact widespread patterns of
- 16:46connectivity in the human brain.
- 16:48So here we see increases in estradiol
- 16:50over time are associated with
- 16:52greater functional connectivity
- 16:53across much of the cortical mantle.
- 16:58Here I'm showing that same finding,
- 17:00just a slightly more stringent threshold.
- 17:03So hotter colors here indicate increased
- 17:07coherence with higher hormone concentrations.
- 17:10Cooler colors indicate the reverse.
- 17:13So one thing you might notice is that
- 17:15you know in contrast to estradiol
- 17:17sort of proliferative effects,
- 17:19we see that progesterone is associated with
- 17:22reduced coherence across the whole brain.
- 17:24And this although it's on a totally
- 17:27different scale fits really beautifully
- 17:29with the rodent work where we know that
- 17:32estradiol tends to be proliferative and
- 17:33progesterone tends to be sort of inhibitory.
- 17:36So we're seeing that on a completely
- 17:38different scale but but the sort
- 17:40of direction of the patterns are
- 17:41it's interesting to note those.
- 17:43Salaries.
- 17:45So next Laura calculated mean Nodal
- 17:48association strengths by intrinsic networks.
- 17:52So here positive just refers to the average
- 17:55magnitude of those positive associations,
- 17:58so stronger coherence with higher estradiol
- 18:01and negative refers to the average
- 18:04magnitude of the inverse association,
- 18:07so weak or coherence with higher estradiol.
- 18:10So you can see that these associations are
- 18:13are evident throughout the major networks,
- 18:16perhaps particularly in dorsal
- 18:20attention and temporoparietal.
- 18:24So we dug in a little bit more.
- 18:27So next we used time lag methods
- 18:29to start to discern the temporal
- 18:32directionality of these associations.
- 18:34So is it estradiol that's driving
- 18:36changes in brain state or might
- 18:39it be the other way around?
- 18:41And so in an analysis led by Tyler Santander,
- 18:44we used vector Autoregression or VAR models
- 18:47to just solve for two basic equations.
- 18:49So is like today's resting
- 18:52state functional connectivity.
- 18:53Um, you know,
- 18:54best.
- 18:55Determined by the pattern of
- 18:57yesterday's functional connectivity.
- 18:59Yesterday's estradiol levels were then
- 19:01you can go sort of to a lag of two back.
- 19:04And similarly we can solve for estradiol
- 19:06in the same way to try to get I'm
- 19:08not going to use the word causal,
- 19:10but to try to understand how these
- 19:13relationships are directed in time.
- 19:16And overwhelmingly we find evidence
- 19:18that it is estradiol.
- 19:20Previous states of estradiol driving
- 19:22current states of the brain,
- 19:24so driving this increase in
- 19:26functional coherence.
- 19:27And here we start to see really
- 19:30default mode control dorsal
- 19:31attention networks being the most
- 19:34strongly associated with estradiol.
- 19:38You know, we can use common
- 19:40graph theory metrics to start to
- 19:41characterize the network properties.
- 19:43So here I'm just going to show
- 19:45one example looking at within
- 19:46network connectivity or efficiency.
- 19:51And we find.
- 19:52So just to Orient you here,
- 19:55estradiol is being plotted
- 19:56by day of experiments.
- 19:58So to the best of her abilities,
- 19:59Laura did this blind.
- 20:00So she started randomly at
- 20:01a certain part of the cycle.
- 20:03So we're just, I'm plotting estrogen
- 20:06based on day of experiment,
- 20:07not day of cycle.
- 20:08And what you can see here is a
- 20:11measure of within the default mode,
- 20:13this measure of global efficiency.
- 20:15And what you see is that when estradiol
- 20:18levels peak right around ovulation,
- 20:21you see this increase in the
- 20:22efficiency of default mode network.
- 20:24When estradiol levels plummet,
- 20:26so too do you see this decrease
- 20:29in the efficiency in this network.
- 20:31And these data,
- 20:32I don't have time to get into it,
- 20:34but I referenced the paper down here.
- 20:36So that replicated in the follow-up
- 20:39study conducted one year later.
- 20:41And just a quick plug,
- 20:43I don't have time to do the data justice,
- 20:45but Rick Betzel's group did a
- 20:47really awesome Edge Time series
- 20:49analysis of this data set,
- 20:51finding that the frequency of
- 20:53high amplitude network states
- 20:55are associated with various HPG
- 20:57access hormones as well.
- 20:58And so here's a plug for that paper.
- 21:04And I think I'm going to skip over
- 21:06this stuff in the interest of time.
- 21:11Because I want to make this point.
- 21:14Intrinsic fluctuations in sex
- 21:16hormones are associated with patterns
- 21:19of brain coherence in both sexes.
- 21:21So I do not want you to walk away
- 21:24from this talk thinking that the
- 21:27menstrual cycle is, you know,
- 21:29pick up a newspaper and you'll hear things
- 21:31like wreaking havoc on the female brain.
- 21:33That couldn't be farther from the truth.
- 21:34Hormones are a feature of males and females,
- 21:38and these, you know,
- 21:40rhythmic pulses exist in both sexes.
- 21:42So here we ran a follow-up study.
- 21:45The densely sampled male and this
- 21:47time the participant underwent
- 21:49brain imaging and venepuncture to do
- 21:52serology every 12 to 24 hours across
- 21:5530 days so that we could target these
- 21:58diurnal changes in steroid hormones.
- 22:00So during this AM to PM diurnal cycle,
- 22:03we see about a 60% change
- 22:05in testosterone production,
- 22:07a 40% change in estradiol.
- 22:12And just like before,
- 22:13we found that increases in testosterone
- 22:16and estradiol in the male brain is
- 22:18associated with increases in whole
- 22:21brain functional coherence overtime.
- 22:23And the overall magnitude of these
- 22:26relationships in the densely
- 22:28sampled male was comparable to
- 22:30the densely sampled female.
- 22:34So, you know, as Rebecca
- 22:37Shansky wrote in an editorial,
- 22:39this is about the animal literature,
- 22:41but it applies here. You know,
- 22:43our hormones a female problem for research?
- 22:46Absolutely not. And I can get into that
- 22:48later in the discussion if you want.
- 22:50But you know, these are are features
- 22:53of of all genders and all sexes.
- 22:56OK. Next we shifted gears
- 22:58to look at brain morphology.
- 22:59So can we see dynamic changes in
- 23:02brain volume across the cycle?
- 23:04So this analysis is led by
- 23:05Caitlin Taylor of my group.
- 23:07So she used high resolution T2
- 23:09imaging to look at the volume
- 23:11of different sub regions of the
- 23:14hippocampus and the surrounding
- 23:16tissue across the menstrual cycle.
- 23:18And then she reran the experiment.
- 23:21But this time the participant was on
- 23:23a drug that chronically suppressed
- 23:26progesterone levels by 97%.
- 23:28So notice that this increase in
- 23:31progesterone across the spontaneous
- 23:33or natural menstrual cycle is
- 23:35absent in the second study,
- 23:37whilst estrogen dynamics were the same.
- 23:42And.
- 23:44If anybody,
- 23:44I don't have the comments turned on,
- 23:46but if anybody can guess what.
- 23:49Medication she was on.
- 23:50What drug she was on.
- 23:51It's the most common form of
- 23:53the oral hormonal contraceptive
- 23:55on the market today.
- 23:59So what do we find across
- 24:01the menstrual cycle?
- 24:02We see that intrinsic fluctuations
- 24:04in progesterone are associated with
- 24:06volumetric changes in several regions.
- 24:09Here I'm just showing two.
- 24:11So when progesterone concentrations
- 24:13are elevated in the luteal phase,
- 24:16here we see increased or an
- 24:18expansion of Gray matter volume in
- 24:21hippocampal C23 and parahippocampal
- 24:22cortex and then if you chronically
- 24:25suppress progesterone as we did.
- 24:27In the second experiment,
- 24:29you abolish this effect.
- 24:34That's just the same thing.
- 24:37So, you know, I hope that this
- 24:40begins to convince you that these
- 24:42neuroendocrine transition states,
- 24:44you know, are having fairly rapid or
- 24:46dynamic effects on the human brain,
- 24:48both structural and functional level.
- 24:50But you know, the endocrine changes
- 24:53that we see across the menstrual cycle
- 24:56are dwarfed by the kinds of endocrine
- 24:59changes that happen during pregnancy.
- 25:01So this really LED us to
- 25:04ask the next question,
- 25:06which is how is the brain reorganizing
- 25:10or changing across this utterly
- 25:13fascinating window of time?
- 25:15And we were really inspired by
- 25:18there's a great paper by Hexana Elson,
- 25:21hexamer and colleagues and a group
- 25:24in Spain looking at comparing the
- 25:26brain preconception and then post
- 25:28delivery and finding all of these
- 25:30interesting Gray matter volume.
- 25:32Differences,
- 25:33specifically within the default mode network.
- 25:37But we were interested in in sort
- 25:39of mapping on the sort of endocrine
- 25:41drivers and the time course with
- 25:42which these changes were happening,
- 25:44which really means we have to
- 25:45apply this dense sampling lens.
- 25:47So this is an ongoing study,
- 25:50but we recently wrapped data
- 25:51collection on on a single individual.
- 25:53So I'm going to show those data here.
- 25:55So we followed a woman starting
- 25:58preconception with MRI's and
- 26:00serological evaluations about
- 26:01every two weeks across the complete
- 26:04gestational window and we have
- 26:06follow-up scans up to two years.
- 26:08Postpartum and I'm I'm gonna save
- 26:10those data for a little bit later,
- 26:12but you can see that sex hormone
- 26:15concentrations increase across
- 26:16the course of pregnancy.
- 26:18So we're putting estrogen and
- 26:20progesterone here and then there's this
- 26:23precipitous drop after the delivery.
- 26:26And across pregnancy,
- 26:27we see changes in medial temporal
- 26:30lobe morphology here plotting
- 26:33parahippocampal cortex with sort of
- 26:36decrease in volume tied both to this
- 26:38change in estrogen and progesterone.
- 26:40Also you can plot it by just
- 26:43gestational week.
- 26:44Suggesting that this might be
- 26:46this period in which there are
- 26:48organizational effects of sex hormones
- 26:51across this gestational period.
- 26:53So again,
- 26:53I hope you're beginning to get a sense of,
- 26:55you know,
- 26:56how responsive the brain appears to be
- 26:58to these major endocrine transitions.
- 27:01And if you're at SRCD in a couple of
- 27:04weeks or CNS a couple days after that,
- 27:07stay tuned because the lab will
- 27:09present some more of the sort of
- 27:12multimodal findings from this project.
- 27:14Including crazy results looking
- 27:16at cortical thickness changes and
- 27:19just the functional connectome
- 27:21changes across this period of time.
- 27:26And you know I'll just say quickly
- 27:30that they're going to menopause later
- 27:32in life is another period of you
- 27:35know major and and neuroendocrine
- 27:37transition where steroid hormone
- 27:40production of estrogen progesterone
- 27:42levels declines by about 90% and you
- 27:45see this increase in the gonadotropin
- 27:48follicle stimulating hormone which
- 27:50increases fairly dramatically
- 27:52during this period of time we see.
- 27:55Seasonal. Let me move, boish.
- 28:00Does anybody hear that?
- 28:07OK, I'm going to keep going,
- 28:08but I heard somebody talking.
- 28:10So I'm just going to end by saying that,
- 28:12um, so, you know.
- 28:14We see changes in these same subfield
- 28:17regions as a function of endocrine aging,
- 28:21and I'll save those data for
- 28:23later and let me just end on this
- 28:26little bit of a soapbox point.
- 28:28This is from an opening of a
- 28:30special issue of Frontiers and their
- 28:33endocrinology that Lisa Gallia and
- 28:36Annmarie Delange and I put together.
- 28:39And we write that, you know,
- 28:41neuroscience has overlooked
- 28:42aspects of the human condition,
- 28:44whether talking about the menstrual cycle,
- 28:45the pill, pregnancy, menopause.
- 28:47That's relevant to half
- 28:49of the world's population.
- 28:51And half of the US tax base and
- 28:54we've got to correct course
- 28:56us for the field to advance.
- 28:58And just to put some some data to
- 29:01that as Nora Imager as you've all
- 29:04witnessed this stunning growth in
- 29:05neuroimaging studies of the human
- 29:07brain over the last 30 years,
- 29:09let's call it the blue wave.
- 29:11In yellow as a reference point,
- 29:13you can see the number of brain
- 29:16imaging publications on depression.
- 29:17And then if you squint really hard,
- 29:20you might be able to detect the
- 29:22black line at the bottom.
- 29:24This is the totality of articles
- 29:27covering whole suite of factors,
- 29:29including menopause, pregnancy,
- 29:30hormonal birth control,
- 29:31the menstrual cycle, and much, much more.
- 29:34So altogether,
- 29:35studies on these Women's Health
- 29:37factors constitute less than half of
- 29:391% of the total brain imaging literature.
- 29:43And I think by ignoring these factors,
- 29:45we risk making two major mistakes.
- 29:47First, the brain is an endocrine organ,
- 29:49and yet we have almost no insight
- 29:51into how dynamic changes in sex
- 29:53hormones shape the human brain at,
- 29:55you know,
- 29:56the mesoscopic and macroscopic scales
- 29:58discernible through brain imaging.
- 30:00And second,
- 30:01we're missing critical clues about
- 30:03why these endocrine transition
- 30:05states may be periods of resiliency
- 30:07or vulnerability, right?
- 30:09Think about postpartum depression,
- 30:10perimenopausal depression,
- 30:11the fact that women make up
- 30:122/3 of the Alzheimer's disease.
- 30:14Population.
- 30:15You know,
- 30:15right now I think the human brain
- 30:17imaging community is underserving women.
- 30:20Or to put it in a more positive light,
- 30:22let's say progress in neuroscience
- 30:24will flourish when when the health
- 30:26of men and women are valued equally.
- 30:29So I'm a firm believer in Katarinas's
- 30:32model of kind of go big or go home.
- 30:36And so we're taking that to heart.
- 30:38My lab does a lot of these sort of,
- 30:40you know,
- 30:41small and focused studies to map the
- 30:43brain across these transition states.
- 30:45But we also want to to take
- 30:49the other approach.
- 30:50And last month,
- 30:51we secured funding to launch the and
- 30:53Bowers women's Brain Health initiative.
- 30:55And the idea is to take
- 30:57Women's Health prime time.
- 30:58And at the University of California,
- 30:59I think we can do that through
- 31:02deeply collaborative science.
- 31:03So across the University
- 31:04of California system,
- 31:05we have eight brain imaging
- 31:07centers dedicated for research.
- 31:08These centers generate data from
- 31:10thousands of participants annually,
- 31:11and so we're capitalizing on that
- 31:14activity by creating a population level.
- 31:16Open Access brain imaging database
- 31:19designed unabashedly and specifically to
- 31:21strengthening Women's Health research,
- 31:23so pooling data collected
- 31:25across each of these sites.
- 31:28And Russ has graciously agreed to
- 31:30serve as our data coordinating center.
- 31:33So this is all quite new and and in
- 31:35fact I will probably be reaching out
- 31:37to a number of you in the audience
- 31:39to be part of our Executive Advisory
- 31:41Board as we as we grow this baby,
- 31:44but it is now happening and I'm thrilled.
- 31:47And with that,
- 31:48I'll just give thanks to all of the
- 31:51people who actually did this work,
- 31:53including the biggest shoutout
- 31:54to the one in the middle,
- 31:55Laura Pritchett for leading
- 31:57many of these studies.
- 31:59And with that,
- 32:00I will take questions.
- 32:07Thank you so much. Do we have any questions?
- 32:11Yeah.
- 32:14So great talk. Thank you.
- 32:15Very super interesting.
- 32:18I had a question not about Women's
- 32:21Health perceive but more about the time
- 32:24dependency of connectivity in other words.
- 32:27My tendency mean people get scanned
- 32:29in the morning versus afternoon and
- 32:31I have not seen that controlled
- 32:33course of compound any kind of
- 32:35any study that I've ever reading.
- 32:36Do you think that difference is
- 32:39significant enough for you to start
- 32:41worrying about it looking at it closely?
- 32:44Thomas's group had you know,
- 32:45time of day paper out,
- 32:47so you should pick his brain about that.
- 32:49So yeah, I think.
- 32:53I think I would certainly consider
- 32:55it as a factor and make sure that
- 32:57it is not systematically biased by
- 33:00the factor that you're interested
- 33:02in or by your groups.
- 33:04You know,
- 33:04I think one of the reasons you know
- 33:08that this question came up was,
- 33:11you know,
- 33:11doesn't matter where a woman is in
- 33:13her menstrual cycle when you scan her.
- 33:15And you know by chance you're going
- 33:18to have hopefully some kind of random
- 33:20distribution which may wash out your effect.
- 33:23But I think what these data suggests
- 33:25is that to a certain extent,
- 33:27during certain epochs,
- 33:28certainly during that ovulatory period,
- 33:30we are seeing pretty striking changes,
- 33:33you know?
- 33:34Happening of either the morphological
- 33:36and the functional level and.
- 33:38Here I'm just trying to find this slide.
- 33:43You know in the the 20 and he data set is,
- 33:46you know, suggesting too that at least
- 33:48whether it's time of day driven by
- 33:50something else or whether it's, you know,
- 33:53these clear diurnal changes in hormones
- 33:55that these are also affecting properties
- 33:58of the functional connectome so.
- 34:01You know, we always time lock our scans,
- 34:04but that's because, you know,
- 34:05we're interested in these
- 34:06changing endocrine functions.
- 34:07So I don't know whether I want
- 34:09to advise you to do the same,
- 34:10but it's certainly something that I
- 34:11would pay attention to in your data set.
- 34:17Sorry, I can't see who's asking questions.
- 34:20OK. I have a question 2 actually.
- 34:22One is did you collect any
- 34:25behavioral data in the 28 and me?
- 34:27And then the second question,
- 34:29now that you're going you know,
- 34:31more large scale and how are
- 34:33you going to control or maybe
- 34:36look at depending on you know,
- 34:38your perspective of the individual variation
- 34:40and you know hormonal fluctuations
- 34:42when you are now looking at you know?
- 34:45Thousands of people.
- 34:47Yeah, great.
- 34:48Great questions.
- 34:49So in the first in Laura's 28 and me study,
- 34:53we got that out into the field very
- 34:58quickly and did not spend the time
- 35:02necessary to think about how we could
- 35:06collect behavioral data that we were
- 35:08interested in or that we thought
- 35:10could be robust to 30 days of testing.
- 35:13We could have done it,
- 35:14we just didn't.
- 35:15So we have if you look at.
- 35:17Her paper we have.
- 35:20Characterize sort of mood changes
- 35:23and you know sleep etcetera.
- 35:25So just state changes in those,
- 35:27but we don't have very rich assessments of
- 35:31changes in in cognitive behavior over time.
- 35:35We do have data.
- 35:39So that said, we do have.
- 35:43Data from a selective attention task
- 35:45that she performed in the scanner.
- 35:48So her performance is pretty
- 35:50much near ceiling the whole time.
- 35:52It's not fluctuating,
- 35:53but we do and we haven't actually
- 35:54looked at this data yet,
- 35:55but we do have the ability to
- 35:58look at kind of.
- 35:59The brains response to
- 36:01that paradigm overtime so.
- 36:04We're just swimming in data and
- 36:06haven't gotten a chance to do that yet.
- 36:08And then the second question.
- 36:09Yeah how in the big data set,
- 36:12how do you control for these
- 36:13hormones questions?
- 36:14This is this is the biggest conundrum
- 36:16that we're having right now.
- 36:17I you know, we're we're exploring a lot
- 36:21of different options including just.
- 36:24You know,
- 36:25at UCSB we have a phlebotomist
- 36:26who just lives at the brain
- 36:28imaging center and draws blood on
- 36:29and everybody that we bring in,
- 36:31whether we can do that across
- 36:33all centers is TBD,
- 36:34but we're there's no option
- 36:36that's off the table.
- 36:38So in the pooling of the brain imaging data,
- 36:42we pair that with deidentified
- 36:43health information that looks
- 36:45across the life course to get
- 36:47information about women's.
- 36:50Whether they're natural cycling,
- 36:52whether they're on some kind of hormone based
- 36:55medication features of pregnancy et cetera.
- 36:57So we have those data and now we're
- 37:01we're thinking about ways of pairing
- 37:03it with time locked hormone assessments
- 37:06and saliva unfortunately is no good.
- 37:08So that would be the easiest solution,
- 37:10but it's, it's not great.
- 37:13For what we're interested in.
- 37:16That terrific talk with my
- 37:18friend Lisa Galea was here.
- 37:20She'd be joining you on your
- 37:23soapbox and plotting.
- 37:24That was fantastic.
- 37:25I've got a sort of mechanistic question,
- 37:28I guess.
- 37:30So if you think that the human variation
- 37:36across the cycle in synaptic density,
- 37:39for example, is similar to the mouse
- 37:42variation within the estrous cycle,
- 37:45which is like.
- 37:47We're doing something to something.
- 37:49That that's a lot, yeah.
- 37:51So that's a lot of spine density changes,
- 37:56right.
- 37:58Which means, you know,
- 37:58if you think about C1 or
- 38:00C2 or something like that,
- 38:01that that's a lot of human experience
- 38:04that is being dissolved and then
- 38:07coming back and yet you know we are
- 38:09who we are and we don't change that.
- 38:12You know,
- 38:12there may be behavioral changes but
- 38:14you know our long term memories
- 38:16and so on don't change.
- 38:17So what, what do you think these
- 38:20changes actually subserve?
- 38:21It's kind of a philosophical question
- 38:24but that's a lot of pruning and growth.
- 38:27Without a.
- 38:29Obvious reason for it.
- 38:32And who are you again?
- 38:37Oh, you can't see me. I'm Ravi Menon.
- 38:39I'm a professor at Western University.
- 38:42Yeah. OK. Hi. Yes.
- 38:44Lisa is a dear friend.
- 38:46And and we are. We think about
- 38:48these things a lot together.
- 38:54You know, yeah. So this is sort of
- 38:56like ultimate level of causation,
- 38:58like why are these things happening?
- 39:02You know. It's the $1,000,000 question.
- 39:06I think it it's easier to describe
- 39:09why this might be happening
- 39:11in the rodent models, right?
- 39:13So maybe this is vestigial,
- 39:14maybe this is sort of carryover
- 39:15from just sort of mammalian brain.
- 39:17But, you know, in a rodent and
- 39:20certainly as evidenced in, you know,
- 39:22in some of Norah's work looking at
- 39:24the functional advantages of these,
- 39:26you know this.
- 39:28Affecting place field stability
- 39:31and let's say flexibility,
- 39:34you know I'm,
- 39:35we know that there are brain
- 39:37changes if you are in,
- 39:39you know in seasonally breeding animals
- 39:41across the seasons in Prairie voles.
- 39:44We know that there are these
- 39:46changes during mating seasons
- 39:48because they can traverse then a
- 39:51greater space in the environment.
- 39:53And you see these sort of growth
- 39:55in either sort of you know growth
- 39:57hippocampal volume or spine agenesis.
- 39:59Um,
- 39:59so there it's it clearly has this sort of,
- 40:02you know, functional role where you know,
- 40:05the animals either sourcing food,
- 40:08sourcing mates and they need a
- 40:10neuronal system that can support that.
- 40:13You know why we still see these
- 40:15ebbs and flows and in humans,
- 40:17um, you know that's less clear.
- 40:21But clearly these hormones are
- 40:23responsive not just across the cycle
- 40:26but as I mentioned very briefly,
- 40:29you know across these other endocrine
- 40:31transition states like pregnancy and
- 40:33there I start I think we can start to
- 40:36think about and and I'll just give a plug to.
- 40:39To CNS.
- 40:39If anybody's there,
- 40:40go and take a look at.
- 40:43At the rest of these data.
- 40:44But, you know,
- 40:45here's a period where the brain is preparing,
- 40:49you know, if you take an alibris woman
- 40:51who's never experienced pregnancy before,
- 40:54the brain is preparing for this completely,
- 40:57you know,
- 40:58pretty radical change in function.
- 41:01And what we're seeing is a lot
- 41:03of sculpting and remodeling
- 41:04within the default mode network.
- 41:06This echoes earlier work,
- 41:08as I mentioned from Ellen Hexamers group.
- 41:12So, you know,
- 41:12maybe this is this sort of fine tuning of
- 41:15those sort of theory of mind capabilities,
- 41:17because what more do you need to do
- 41:19as a parent than have the ability
- 41:22to empathize with this hungry,
- 41:24screaming,
- 41:24crying,
- 41:24you know thing and and to try
- 41:27to understand those mental
- 41:29states for the first time.
- 41:33Just a guess, but we'll we'll
- 41:36keep exploring those questions as
- 41:38we dig more and more into this.
- 41:40Data. No, thank you.
- 41:43I also have a question related to this.
- 41:46I'm interested particularly in
- 41:49neuroendocrine neuromodulation.
- 41:50So the results you see with the the
- 41:54direct imaging on the the rats,
- 41:56would they be replicated if we just
- 41:59have like neuronal organoids in vitro
- 42:02and you change the concentration of,
- 42:04I don't know, progesterone and estradiol,
- 42:06would we see this increase in spine
- 42:10numbers you know? This is the very basic.
- 42:15Response cell to changes in the
- 42:18concentration of the modulator?
- 42:20Or is it? I don't know.
- 42:24Great question.
- 42:24I don't know, I, you know I can.
- 42:27Say with some confidence.
- 42:28I don't think those experiments have
- 42:31ever been run in an organoid setup.
- 42:33It would be really,
- 42:35really interesting to do that.
- 42:38Yeah, thank you.
- 42:39Because yeah,
- 42:39there are no another looks.
- 42:43Yeah, I think that's a I think that's
- 42:46a great you know I again I think.
- 42:49These are pretty specific,
- 42:50so it's not like we're seeing,
- 42:52at least in this model we're not seeing.
- 42:56Every cell, right,
- 42:57every pyramidal neuron show these effects.
- 42:59You know these in these sets of
- 43:02experiments we're looking at,
- 43:04you know, CA one neurons, so.
- 43:07TBD on the the sort of extent to which
- 43:10we can see this remapping play in other,
- 43:14you know, regions.
- 43:15OK, at this stage it was only
- 43:17verified in C3 criminal neurons.
- 43:20It wasn't verified.
- 43:21It happens or not in other neurons.
- 43:23That's the thing.
- 43:29OK. Thank you so much.